The use of phonetic, experimental, and laboratory methods to answer phonological questions has exploded in popularity since the 1987 founding of the Association for Laboratory Phonology. As applied to African languages, these sorts of approaches have until recently been used primarily in the pursuit of documentation and description, as opposed to exploration of questions of phonological structure, representation, and theory. In this session, we highlight recent work in this vein—bringing a "laboratory" framework to bear on questions of African phonology.
Descriptive phonetic studies of African languages have a long and rich history—for example, Clement Doke used X-ray photography in his pioneering 1923 study of the posterior constriction of Zulu clicks. Along similarly descriptive lines, acoustic analysis has been used to describe voicing quality in Xhosa clicks (Jessen and Roux 2002) and Kinyarwanda tone (Myers 2003); ultrasound has been employed in the study of N|uu click articulation (Miller et al 2009) and whistled fricatives in Xitsonga (Lee-Kim, Kawahara, and Lee 2014); and MRI has been used to study production of Khoekhoe clicks in real time (Proctor et al. 2016).
There is also a rich and longstanding tradition of African languages making significant contributions to phonological theory. For example, the Obligatory Contour Principle originated from analyses of Mende tone (Leben 1973, Goldsmith 1976). Hyman’s (1985) development of moraic theory is motivated chiefly by data from Idoma, Gokana, and Kpelle. Agreement by Correspondence (ABC) theory was proposed based on evidence from Yaka, Kikongo, Ngbaka, and Ethiopian Semitic languages (Walker 2000, Rose 2000).
Curiously, the intersection of these two bodies of work remained somewhat of a gap in the literature—until recently. Instrumental, quantitative, and experimental methods have highlighted a wide array of factors as relevant to phonological patterns, like frequency and neighborhood effects, fine-grained phonetic details, and so forth. While it has historically been difficult to rigorously examine some of these factors in under-resourced and under-studied African languages, more recent work has increasingly leveraged new technologies to tackle questions at this intersection of phonetics and phonology. For example, Walker et al. (2008) use EMA articulatory data to shed new light on transparency in sibilant harmony. Bennett and Braver (2015) use a wug-test paradigm to test the productivity of labial palatalization in Xhosa. Lionnet (2017) proposes a novel feature theory based on coarticulatory factors that only truly reveal themselves under serious acoustic analysis. These examples serve to show how key insights about phonological patterns may emerge only under the light of additional data of types not traditionally recorded in descriptive grammars.
This session aims to showcase current research that uses “lab” methods to document or quantify phonological patterns in African languages to add illustration to description. Our inspiration here is from the JIPA ‘Illustrations of the IPA’ series, but we see a benefit and necessity in illustrating phonological patterns using lines of data that may fall outside the scope of JIPA’s focus on phonetics.
1:45–1:50
|
Introduction
|
1:50–2:10
|
Acoustics and Aerodynamics of Nghlwa Implosives
Alexandra Pfiffner, Lindsay Hatch, & Katherine R. Russell (UC Berkeley)
Abstract
Implosives are found in 13% of the world’s languages (Maddieson, 1984) and are particularly widespread throughout the languages of sub-Saharan Africa, but they remain extremely understudied in phonetics and phonology (Clements & Osu, 2002). Although canonically described as voiced with an ingressive glottalic airstream, studies show their production is highly variable; for example, implosives can be produced with no ingressive airstream and without rarefaction (negative oral air pressure; Ladefoged, 1968, 1971), and they can be produced with or without full glottal closure (Ladefoged & Maddieson, 1996). In this study, we report our preliminary findings on the acoustics and aerodynamics of implosives in Nghlwa [ISO:gwa], a Kwa language spoken by the M’Bato people in the La Mé Region of Côte d’Ivoire. In fieldwork elicitations, we collected acoustic, electroglottograph, nasal airflow, oral airflow, and oral pressure measurements. We discuss the overall phonetic profile of Nghlwa implosives and how it relates to their split phonological patterning with both obstruents and sonorants.
Collapse
|
2:10–2:30
|
Phonetic and phonological patterning of glottalized sonorants in Lobi
Amber Galvano, Sansan Claude Hien, and Hannah Sande (UC Berkeley)
Abstract
The Lobi (Gur) consonant inventory contains implosive and labiovelar stops as well as plain and glottalized sonorants. Using distributional facts, articulatory EGG data, and acoustic data, we offer several insights into the set of sounds /ɓ, b, j', l', w', j, l, w, kp, ɡb/, including: (i) /ɓ/ shows positional allophony and distinct phonological behavior from /b/; (ii) all glottalized sounds show show semi-periodic supra-glottal activity leading up to segment onset; (iii) bilabial voiced stops and labiovelars pattern alike in voicing onset; (iv) all included sounds tend to have more dramatic phonetic cues present word initially than word medially. Depending on which metrics one uses, different phonetic and phonological natural classes emerge, though on all metrics /ɓ j' l' w'/ pattern together phonetically. This work contributes to our typological understanding of whether the phonetic realization of glottalized sounds is correlated with their phonological patterning (see Sande and Oakley 2023).
Collapse
|
2:30–2:50
|
Non-dorsal vowels: phonetic and phonological evidence from African languages
Matthew Faytak (University at Buffalo)
Abstract
In this talk, we highlight the existence of non-dorsal vowels with consonant-like place features, closely resembling syllabic labial or coronal fricatives or approximants but not phonetically identical to either. Data from west and central African languages provides key evidence for these phonological representations. Non-dorsal vowels’ place features are most often intrinsic (as in Kom, Limbum, Fang, and Lendu) but sometimes arise through spread from the preceding onset consonant to underspecified vowels (as in Lus, Kung, Isu, or Aghem). Articulatory phonetic data (tongue ultrasound, lip video) provides direct evidence of labial and coronal constrictions during the non-dorsal vowels. Further evidence comes from co-occurence restrictions and variable assimilatory patterns between onsets and non-dorsal vowels which are most easily captured as interactions of the major places of the two segments.
Collapse
|
2:50–3:10
|
Pharyngealization in Two Varieties of Toussian
Tajudeen Mamadou Yacoubou (University of Michigan), Anthony Struthers-Young (UC San Diego)
Abstract
This study aims to experimentally establish whether or not pharyngealization, a phonemic property of vowels in Toussian languages (Gur/Mabia, Burkina Faso), is “less strongly pronounced” in Northern Toussian (NT) than in Southern Toussian (ST) as claimed by Prost (1964). Since acoustic expressions of pharyngealization include lower F2, higher F1, and lower F2-F1 (i.e. more compact) (Shahin 1997; Mohamed 2001; Al-Tamimi 2017), it is expected that F2 would be much lower, along with a much higher F1 in ST than in NT per Prost’s claim. Four male speakers of Toussian (2 per variety) were recorded producing a set of carrier phrases containing target words with and without pharyngealized vowels. Preliminary results show that the mean Bark-converted F2 is indeed overall much lower in ST than in NT, with even more pronounced difference in the second half of the vowel. F1 results were more nuanced. The full analysis includes the vowels [i,ɪ,e,œ,a,o,ɔ].
Collapse
|
3:10–3:15 |
Discussion |
3:15–3:30 |
Break |
3:30–3:40 |
Discussion |
3:40–4:00
|
The acoustic properties of implosives in Guébie (Kru)
Madeleine Oakley and Hannah Sande (UC Berkeley)
Abstract
Implosives have variable cross-linguistic behavior (Sande and Oakley 2023), wherein they can pattern phonologically with obstruents, sonorants, or both in different contexts. This study compares the acoustic properties of implosives to sonorants and obstruents in Guébie (Kru, Côte d’Ivoire), in order to examine whether implosives share phonetic properties with the natural class of sonorants, as predicted by their phonological behavior.
Five Guébie speakers were recorded in Gnagbodougnoa, Côte d’Ivoire. 1,364 voiced obstruents, 1,993 sonorants, and 656 implosives were included in the present analysis.
Implosives, sonorants, and voiced obstruents all differ in duration and intensity. Implosives have a positive intensity slope, whereas sonorants and voiced obstruents have a negative intensity slope. Implosives also have a longer voicing duration ratio than voiced obstruents.
Implosives seem to have acoustic measurements in-between sonorants and obstruents, which, if true across languages, may explain their variable cross-linguistic phonological patterning.
Collapse
|
4:00–4:20
|
Click perception experiments in Xhosa and Zulu
Will Bennett, Aaron Braver , Tyler Miller, Khethani Yende, Camilla Christie (Rhodes University and Texas Tech University)
Abstract
While click consonants have been the object of more than a century of articulatory and acoustic study, there is a relative dearth of literature on their perception. Ladefoged & Traill (1994) present a small-scale experiment about perceptability of clicks over background noise, and find that clicks are considerably easier to identify than non-cliks – presumably due to their extremely high intensity. Pioneering studies conducted by Best et al. (1981, 2003, et seq) find that clicks are on the whole extremely reliably perceived – even by speakers unfamiliar with them. However, descriptive work on South African languages shows rampant variation, abundant doublets, and distributional patterns that suggest that click distinctions are perhaps not so easy and reliable distinguish outside of the lab. In this talk, we present results from several preliminary experiments on click perception with Zulu and Xhosa listeners.
Collapse
|
4:20–4:40
|
Acoustic and Phonology of Gbagyi Tones
Samuel Akinbo & Abigail Dalhatu (University of Toronto)
Abstract
Previous research on Gbagyi (Nupoid, Nigeria) posits three contrastive tones, namely H(igh), L(ow), and M(id) (Hyman and Mogaji 1970). In certain bisyllabic words, the second tones surface with falling and rising contours when the initial tone is higher or lower than the second tone. In this study, we examine the acoustics of tones in the language, which has not been done previously.
In addition to the three lexical tones, we present evidence for a fourth lexically contrastive level
tone, as illustrated with the minimal set [ snì ] “to fetch”, [ snū ] “to roast”, [ sní ] “drink” and [ sni̋ ] “drank”, and the minimal pair [wje̋gje̋] "next year" and [wjégjé] "seeing". Acoustically,
the pitch of the fourth tone is higher than that of the H tone and patterns differently from other
tones in the language. Results of our acoustic study also confirm the previous impressionistic
observation regarding tone transfer. However, we find no evidence to suggest that tone transfer
is optional in the language.
Collapse
|
4:40–5:00 |
Discussion |